/[webpac2]/branches/Sack/t/data/ebsco.txt
This is repository of my old source code which isn't updated any more. Go to git.rot13.org for current projects!
ViewVC logotype

Annotation of /branches/Sack/t/data/ebsco.txt

Parent Directory Parent Directory | Revision Log Revision Log


Revision 1087 - (hide annotations)
Mon Jan 28 17:53:57 2008 UTC (16 years, 5 months ago) by dpavlin
Original Path: trunk/t/data/ebsco.txt
File MIME type: text/plain
File size: 64969 byte(s)
EBSCO text export example file

1 dpavlin 1087 Back
2     <javascript:__doPostBack('ctl00$MainContentArea$deliveryPrintSaveControl$backButton$lnkBack','')>
3    
4     19 article(s) will be saved.
5    
6     To continue, in Internet Explorer, select * FILE* then * SAVE AS* from
7     your browser's toolbar above. Be sure to save as a plain text file
8     (.txt) or a 'Web Page, HTML only' file (.html). In Netscape, select *
9     FILE* then * SAVE AS* from your browser's toolbar above.
10    
11    
12     Record: 1
13    
14     TI- How to learn the natural numbers: Inductive inference and the acquisition of number concepts.
15     AU- Margolis, Eric
16     AU- Laurence, Stephen
17     AF- Margolis, Eric, eamargolis@wisc.edu, Department of Philosophy, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, US
18     AF- Laurence, Stephen, Department of Philosophy, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, US
19     AD- Margolis, Eric, Department of Philosophy, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI, US, 53706, eamargolis@wisc.edu
20     SO- Cognition
21     VI- 106
22     IP- 2
23     YR- 2008
24     PM- Feb 2008
25     PG- 924-939
26     SP- 924
27     PU- Netherlands: Elsevier Science
28     SN- 0010-0277, Print;
29     LA- English
30     KP- natural numbers
31     KP- inference
32     KP- acquisition number concept acquisition
33     KP- induction
34     KP- cognition
35     AB- Theories of number concepts often suppose that the natural numbers are acquired as children learn to count and as they draw an induction based on their interpretation of the first few count words. In a bold critique of this general approach, Rips, Asmuth, Bloomfield [Rips, L., Asmuth, J. & Bloomfield, A. (2006). Giving the boot to the bootstrap: How not to learn the natural numbers. Cognition, 101, B51-B60.] argue that such an inductive inference is consistent with a representational system that clearly does not express the natural numbers and that possession of the natural numbers requires further principles that make the inductive inference superfluous. We argue that their critique is unsuccessful. Provided that children have access to a suitable initial system of representation, the sort of inductive inference that Rips et al. call into question can in fact facilitate the acquisition of larger integer concepts without the addition of any further principles. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
36     MJ- Cognition; Concept Formation; Inference; Numbers (Numerals)
37     CL- Cognitive Processes (2340)
38     PO- Human (10)
39     PT- Journal
40     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
41     AT- Journal Article
42     MT- Electronic
43     MA- Electronic; Print
44     RD- 20080114
45     AN- 2008-00053-016
46     FR- Y
47     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2008-00053-016&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
48    
49     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
50    
51    
52     Record: 2
53    
54     TI- Postscript: Deviations from the predictions of serial search.
55     AU- Adelman, James S.
56     AU- Brown, Gordon D. A.
57     AF- Adelman, James S., University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom
58     AF- Brown, Gordon D. A., University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom
59     SO- Psychological Review
60     VI- 115
61     IP- 1
62     YR- 2008
63     PM- Jan 2008
64     PG- 228-229
65     SP- 228
66     PU- US: American Psychological Association
67     P2- New York, NY,US: Macmillan & Company
68     P2- Princeton, NJ,US: Psychological Review Company
69     P2- Lancaster, PA,US: The Macmillan Company
70     P2- Lancaster, PA,US: The Review Publishing Company
71     SN- 0033-295X, Print; 1939-1471, Electronic;
72     DO- 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.228
73     LA- English
74     KP- lexical decision
75     KP- word frequency
76     KP- contextual diversity
77     KP- bootstrap
78     KP- mathematical modeling
79     KP- word recognition
80     KP- frequency
81     KP- rank, eye movements
82     AB- W. S. Murray and K. I. Forster (see record 2004-15929-006) claimed that rank frequency provided a better account of lexical decision times than either log frequency or power law frequency, the latter being dismissed on the grounds of overflexibility. We (J. S. Adelman & G. D. A. Brown, see record 2008-00265-012) argued that (a) Murray and Forster's (2004) use of the relatively small Kucera and Francis (1967) word frequency counts biased the estimates of rank; (b) the superiority in fit of the power law (and of some other functions) could not all be attributed to overflexibility in the manner Murray and Forster (2004) claimed; and (c) bootstrapping analyses designed to take flexibility into account gave evidence of systematic deviations from several theoretically motivated functional forms, including rank and power, but not from some generalizations of the power function. We concluded that the data could not be taken as support for serial search models. Murray and Forster (2008; see record 2008-00265-015) have suggested that our results do not contradict the rank hypothesis (and in fact support it). The systematic and task-independent discrepancy between model predictions and data suggests to us--in the absence of an extended model demonstrated to rectify the discrepancy--that the case for serial search has yet to be adequately made. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved)
83     MJ- Lexical Access; Lexical Decision; Mathematical Modeling; Word Frequency
84     CL- Cognitive Processes (2340)
85     PO- Human (10)
86     GR- This work was supported by Grant RES 062-23-0545 from the Economic and Social Research Council (UK)
87     PT- Journal
88     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
89     AT- Comment/Reply
90     MT- Electronic
91     MA- Electronic; Print
92     RD- 20080121
93     AN- 2008-00265-023
94     FR- Y
95     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2008-00265-023&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
96    
97     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
98    
99    
100     Record: 3
101    
102     TI- Emotional processing in experiential therapy: Why 'the only way out is through.'
103     AU- Pascual-Leone, Antonio
104     AU- Greenberg, Leslie S.
105     AF- Pascual-Leone, Antonio, apl@uwindsor.ca, Department of Psychology, University of Windsor, Windsor, ON, Canada
106     AF- Greenberg, Leslie S., Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
107     AD- Pascual-Leone, Antonio, Department of Psychology, University of Windsor, 401 Sunset, Windsor, ON, Canada, N9B 3P4, apl@uwindsor.ca
108     SO- Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
109     S2- Journal of Consulting Psychology
110     VI- 75
111     IP- 6
112     YR- 2007
113     PM- Dec 2007
114     PG- 875-887
115     SP- 875
116     PU- US: American Psychological Association
117     P2- Colorado Springs, CO,US: American Association for Applied Psychology
118     P2- Colorado Springs, CO,US: Dentan Printing Company
119     P2- Lancaster, PA,US: Science Press Printing Company
120     SN- 0022-006X, Print; 1939-2117, Electronic;
121     DO- 10.1037/0022-006X.75.6.875
122     LA- English
123     KP- emotional processing
124     KP- emotion-focused therapy
125     KP- distress
126     KP- task analysis
127     KP- change mechanisms
128     KP- experiential therapy
129     AB- The purpose of this study was to examine observable moment-by-moment steps in emotional processing as they occurred within productive sessions of experiential therapy. Global distress was identified as an unprocessed emotion with high arousal and low meaningfulness. The investigation consisted of 2 studies as part of a task analysis that examined clients processing distress in live video-recorded therapy sessions. Clients in both studies were adults in experiential therapy for depression and ongoing interpersonal problems. Study 1 was the discovery-oriented phase of task analysis, which intensively examined 6 examples of global distress. The qualitative findings produced a model showing: global distress, fear, shame, and aggressive anger as undifferentiated and insufficiently processed emotions; the articulation of needs and negative self-evaluations as a pivotal step in change; and assertive anger, self-soothing, hurt, and grief as states of advanced processing. Study 2 tested the model using a sample of 34 clients in global distress. A multivariate analysis of variance showed that the model of emotional processing predicted positive in-session effects, and bootstrapping analyses were used to demonstrate that distinct emotions emerged moment by moment in predicted sequential patterns. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
130     MJ- Emotion Focused Therapy; Emotional Regulation; Experiential Psychotherapy; Treatment Outcomes
131     MN- Distress; Stages of Change
132     CL- Psychotherapy & Psychotherapeutic Counseling (3310)
133     PO- Human (10)
134     PO- Male (30)
135     PO- Female (40)
136     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
137     AG- Young Adulthood (18-29 yrs) (320)
138     AG- Thirties (30-39 yrs) (340)
139     AG- Middle Age (40-64 yrs) (360)
140     TM- Classification of Affective-Meaning States
141     TM- Client Experiencing Scale
142     MD- Empirical Study
143     MD- Quantitative Study
144     PT- Journal
145     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
146     AT- Journal Article
147     MT- Electronic
148     MA- Electronic; Print
149     RD- 20071217
150     AN- 2007-19013-005
151     FR- Y
152     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-19013-005&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
153    
154     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
155    
156    
157     Record: 4
158    
159     TI- Bipolar pharmacotherapy and suicidal behavior. Part I: Lithium, divalproex and carbamazepine.
160     AU- Yerevanian, Boghos I.
161     AU- Koek, Ralph J.
162     AU- Mintz, Jim
163     AF- Yerevanian, Boghos I., byerevan@ucla.edu, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, US
164     AF- Koek, Ralph J., Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, US
165     AF- Mintz, Jim, Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, US
166     AD- Yerevanian, Boghos I., 16111 Plummer Street (116A-11), North Hills, CA, US, 91343, byerevan@ucla.edu
167     SO- Journal of Affective Disorders
168     VI- 103
169     IP- 1
170     YR- 2007
171     PM- Nov 2007
172     PG- 5-11
173     SP- 5
174     PU- Netherlands: Elsevier Science
175     SN- 0165-0327, Print;
176     DO- 10.1016/j.jad.2007.05.019
177     LA- English
178     KP- bipolar pharmacotherapy
179     KP- suicidal behavior
180     KP- lithium
181     KP- divalprex
182     KP- carbamazepine
183     KP- public health
184     AB- Introduction: The anti-suicidal benefit of lithium on suicidal behavior in bipolar disorder is well-established. Data are mixed on the effects of divalproex and carbamazepine. Methods: Retrospective chart review study of 405 veterans with bipolar disorder followed for a mean of 3 years, with month by month review of clinical progress notes, and systematic assessment of current pharmacotherapy and suicide completion, attempt or hospitalization for suicidality. Comparison of suicide event rates (events/100 patient years) between mood stabilizers and during-vs-after discontinuation of mood stabilizers, with linear regression analysis for influence of potential confounding variables, and robust bootstrap confirmation analysis. Results: No completed suicides occurred during or after discontinuation of monotherapy. Rates of non-lethal suicidal behavior were similar during lithium (2.49), divalproex (4.67) and carbamazepine (3.80) monotherapies. There was a sixteen fold greater, highly statistically significant non-lethal suicidal event rate after discontinuation compared with during mood stabilizer monotherapy (55.89 vs. 3.48 events/100 patient years; Chi²=13.95; df=1; p<0.0002). On compared with off treatment differences were similar for the three different agents. Limitations: Treatments were uncontrolled in this naturalistic setting, and data were analyzed retrospectively. Conclusions: Lithium and the anticonvulsants may show similar benefits in protecting bipolar patients from non-lethal suicidal behavior when careful analysis of clinical data is done to confirm medication adherence/non-adherence. Findings in this study were similar to those of a previous study that applied the same methodology in a private practice setting. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
185     MJ- Attempted Suicide; Bipolar Disorder; Drug Therapy; Drugs; Suicide
186     MN- Carbamazepine; Lithium; Public Health
187     CL- Psychological & Physical Disorders (3200)
188     CL- Clinical Psychopharmacology (3340)
189     PO- Human (10)
190     PO- Male (30)
191     PO- Female (40)
192     LO- US
193     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
194     AG- Middle Age (40-64 yrs) (360)
195     GR- The study was partially funded by Abbott Laboratories
196     MD- Empirical Study
197     MD- Longitudinal Study
198     MD- Retrospective Study
199     MD- Quantitative Study
200     PT- Journal
201     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
202     AT- Journal Article
203     MT- Electronic
204     MA- Electronic; Print
205     RD- 20071029
206     AN- 2007-14984-002
207     FR- Y
208     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-14984-002&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
209    
210     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
211    
212    
213     Record: 5
214    
215     TI- Abnormal reliance on object structure in apraxics' learning on novel object-related actions.
216     AU- Barde, Laura H. F.
217     AU- Buxbaum, Laurel J.
218     AU- Moll, Adrienne D.
219     AF- Barde, Laura H. F., bardel@einstein.edu, Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Philadelphia, PA, US
220     AF- Buxbaum, Laurel J., Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Philadelphia, PA, US
221     AF- Moll, Adrienne D., Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Philadelphia, PA, US
222     AD- Barde, Laura H. F., Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Korman Suite 213, 1200 West Tabor Road, Philadelphia, PA, US, 19141, bardel@einstein.edu
223     SO- Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society
224     VI- 13
225     IP- 6
226     YR- 2007
227     PM- Nov 2007
228     PG- 997-1008
229     SP- 997
230     PU- US: Cambridge Univ Press
231     SN- 1355-6177, Print; 1469-7661, Electronic;
232     LA- English
233     KP- abnormal reliance
234     KP- object structure
235     KP- apraxics learning
236     KP- novel object related actions
237     KP- ideomotor apraxia
238     AB- We assessed the prediction that object structural cues could benefit the learning of object-action relationships in ideomotor apraxia (IMA). A total of 15 patients with left-hemisphere stroke, 11 of whom exhibited IMA, and 10 healthy subjects were trained to match novel gestures to novel tool pictures that were either High- or Low-Afforded by their associated tools. Learning was assessed with recognition and production tests. Only IMA patients demonstrated better recognition of High- than Low-Afforded gestures, and their recognition of High-Afforded gestures was statistically comparable to the other groups. This finding suggests that apraxics may rely abnormally on object structure when learning to associate novel gestures and tools. Finally, the "affordance benefit" was associated with relative sparing of structures in the dorsal visual processing stream. These data are consistent with the proposal that two routes may mediate skilled action, one specialized for stored information, and the other responsive to object structure, and that deficient gesture learning may be compensated by "bootstrapping" intact dorsal stream coding of action. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
239     MJ- Apraxia; Learning; Object Recognition
240     CL- Neurological Disorders & Brain Damage (3297)
241     PO- Human (10)
242     GR- This work was supported by a University of Utah Funding Incentive Seed Grant awarded to the first author
243     PT- Journal
244     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
245     AT- Journal Article
246     MT- Print
247     MA- Electronic; Print
248     RD- 20071217
249     AN- 2007-17471-010
250     FR- Y
251     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-17471-010&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
252    
253     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
254    
255    
256     Record: 6
257    
258     TI- Visual contribution to the multistable perception of speech.
259     AU- Sato, Marc
260     AU- Basirat, Anahita
261     AU- Schwartz, Jean-Luc
262     AF- Sato, Marc, UMR 5216 CNRS, Institut National Polytechnique de Grenoble, Universite Stendhal, Grenoble, France
263     AF- Basirat, Anahita, UMR 5216 CNRS, Institut National Polytechnique de Grenoble, Universite Stendhal, Grenoble, France
264     AF- Schwartz, Jean-Luc, schwartz@gipsa-lab.inpg.fr, UMR 5216 CNRS, Institut National Polytechnique de Grenoble, Universite Stendhal, Grenoble, France
265     AD- Schwartz, Jean-Luc, GIPSA-Lab, Departement Parole et Cognition, UMR 5216 CNRS, Institut National Polytechnique de Grenoble, Universite Stendahl, 46, Avenue Felix Viallet, 38031, Grenoble, France, Cedex 01, schwartz@gipsa-lab.inpg.fr
266     SO- Perception & Psychophysics
267     VI- 69
268     IP- 8
269     YR- 2007
270     PM- Nov 2007
271     PG- 1360-1372
272     SP- 1360
273     PU- US: Psychonomic Society
274     SN- 0031-5117, Print; 1532-5962, Electronic;
275     LA- English
276     KP- visual contribution
277     KP- multistable perception
278     KP- speech perception
279     KP- verbal transformation effect
280     AB- The multistable perception of speech, or verbal transformation effect, refers to perceptual changes experienced while listening to a speech form that is repeated rapidly and continuously. In order to test whether visual information from the speaker's articulatory gestures may modify the emergence and stability of verbal auditory percepts, subjects were instructed to report any perceptual changes during unimodal, audiovisual, and incongruent audiovisual presentations of distinct repeated syllables. In a first experiment, the perceptual stability of reported auditory percepts was significantly modulated by the modality of presentation. In a second experiment, when audiovisual stimuli consisting of a stable audio track dubbed with a video track that alternated between congruent and incongruent stimuli were presented, a strong correlation between the timing of perceptual transitions and the timing of video switches was found. Finally, a third experiment showed that the vocal tract opening onset event provided by the visual input could play the role of a bootstrap mechanism in the search for transformations. Altogether, these results demonstrate the capacity of visual information to control the multistable perception of speech in its phonetic content and temporal course. The verbal transformation effect thus provides a useful experimental paradigm to explore audiovisual interactions in speech perception. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
281     MJ- Oral Communication; Phonetics; Speech Perception; Visual Perception
282     CL- Sensory Perception (2320)
283     CL- Linguistics & Language & Speech (2720)
284     PO- Human (10)
285     PO- Male (30)
286     PO- Female (40)
287     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
288     AG- Young Adulthood (18-29 yrs) (320)
289     GR- This work was supported by CNRS (Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique) and MIUR (Ministero Italiano dell'Istruzione, dell'Università e della Ricerca)
290     MD- Empirical Study
291     MD- Quantitative Study
292     PT- Journal
293     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
294     AT- Journal Article
295     MT- Print
296     MA- Electronic; Print
297     RD- 20071210
298     AN- 2007-18138-009
299     FR- Y
300     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-18138-009&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
301    
302     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
303    
304    
305     Record: 7
306    
307     TI- Association of synapsin 2 with schizophrenia in families of Northern European ancestry.
308     AU- Saviouk, Viatcheslav
309     AU- Moreau, Michael P.
310     AU- Tereshchenko, Irina V.
311     AU- Brzustowicz, Linda M.
312     AF- Saviouk, Viatcheslav, saviouk@biology.rutgers.edu, Department of Genetics, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, US
313     AF- Moreau, Michael P., Department of Genetics, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, US
314     AF- Tereshchenko, Irina V., Department of Genetics, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, US
315     AF- Brzustowicz, Linda M., Department of Genetics, Rutgers University, Piscataway, NJ, US
316     AD- Saviouk, Viatcheslav, Rutgers University, Department of Genetics, 145 Bevier Road, Room 226, Piscataway, NJ, US, 08854-8000, saviouk@biology.rutgers.edu
317     SO- Schizophrenia Research
318     VI- 96
319     IP- 1
320     YR- 2007
321     PM- Nov 2007
322     PG- 100-111
323     SP- 100
324     PU- Netherlands: Elsevier Science
325     SN- 0920-9964, Print;
326     DO- 10.1016/j.schres.2007.07.031
327     LA- English
328     KP- synapsin 2
329     KP- schizophrenia
330     KP- families
331     KP- Northern European ancestry
332     KP- linkage
333     KP- TRANSMIT
334     KP- PDTPHASE
335     AB- The synapsin 2 (Syn2) gene (3p25) is implicated in synaptogenesis, neurotransmitter release, and the localization of nitric oxide synthase to the proximity of its targets. In this study we investigated linkage and association between the Syn2 locus and schizophrenia. 37 pedigrees of Northern European ancestry from the NIMH Human Genetics Initiative collection were used. Four microsatellites and twenty SNPs were genotyped. Linkage (FASTLINK) and association (TRANSMIT, PDTPHASE) between markers and schizophrenia were evaluated. A maximum heterogeneity LOD of 1.93 was observed at marker D3S3434 with a recessive mode of inheritance. Significant results were obtained for association with schizophrenia using TRANSMIT (minimum nominal p=0.0000005) and PDTPHASE (minimum nominal p=0.014) using single marker analyses. Haplotype analysis using markers in introns 5 and 6 of Syn2 provided a single haplotype that is significantly associated with schizophrenia using TRANSMIT (nominal pb0.00000001) and PDTPHASE (nominal p=0.02). Simulation studies confirm the global significance of these results, but demonstrate that the small p-values generated by the bootstrap routine of TRANSMIT can be consistently anticonservative. Review of the literature suggests that Syn2 is likely to be involved in the etiology or pathogenesis of schizophrenia. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
336     MJ- Etiology; Genes; Genetics; Schizophrenia
337     CL- Schizophrenia & Psychotic States (3213)
338     CL- Genetics (2510)
339     PO- Human (10)
340     PO- Male (30)
341     PO- Female (40)
342     LO- US
343     GR- This work was supported by grant R01 MH62440 from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to LMB
344     MD- Empirical Study
345     MD- Quantitative Study
346     SL- Other [Internet Available]
347     PT- Journal
348     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
349     AT- Journal Article
350     MT- Electronic
351     MA- Electronic; Print
352     RD- 20071029
353     AN- 2007-15151-011
354     FR- Y
355     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-15151-011&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
356    
357     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
358    
359    
360     Record: 8
361    
362     TI- Ego identity, social anxiety, social support, and self-concealment in lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals.
363     AU- Potoczniak, Daniel J.
364     AU- Aldea, Mirela A.
365     AU- DeBlaere, Cirleen
366     AF- Potoczniak, Daniel J., Department of Psychology, University of Florida, FL, US
367     AF- Aldea, Mirela A., Department of Psychology, University of Florida, FL, US
368     AF- DeBlaere, Cirleen, Department of Psychology, University of Florida, FL, US
369     AD- Potoczniak, Daniel J., Counseling and Psychological Services, University of Pennsylvania, 133 S. 36th Street, 2nd Floor, Philadelphia, PA, US, 19104
370     SO- Journal of Counseling Psychology
371     VI- 54
372     IP- 4
373     YR- 2007
374     PM- Oct 2007
375     PG- 447-457
376     SP- 447
377     PU- US: American Psychological Association
378     P2- Dubuque, IA,US: Wm. C. Brown Co.
379     SN- 0022-0167, Print; 1939-2168, Electronic;
380     DO- 10.1037/0022-0167.54.4.447
381     LA- English
382     KP- sexual orientation
383     KP- ego identity
384     KP- social anxiety
385     KP- social support
386     KP- self-concealment
387     AB- This study examined a model in which the relationship between social anxiety and two dimensions of ego identity (commitment and exploration) was expected to be mediated by social support and self-concealment for a sample of lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals (N=347). Statistically significant paths were found from social anxiety to social support and self-concealment. Statistically significant paths were also found from social support to commitment, exploration, and self-concealment. There were no significant paths from social anxiety to commitment or exploration. Structural equation analyses and bootstrap procedures revealed support for the potential mediational role of social support in the association between social anxiety and the two dimensions of ego identity as well as in the link between social anxiety and self-concealment. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
388     MJ- Ego Identity; Sexual Orientation; Social Anxiety; Social Support
389     MN- Bisexuality; Identity Formation; Lesbianism; Male Homosexuality; Secrecy; Self Disclosure
390     CL- Personality Traits & Processes (3120)
391     CL- Sexual Behavior & Sexual Orientation (2980)
392     PO- Human (10)
393     PO- Male (30)
394     PO- Female (40)
395     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
396     AG- Young Adulthood (18-29 yrs) (320)
397     AG- Thirties (30-39 yrs) (340)
398     AG- Middle Age (40-64 yrs) (360)
399     AG- Aged (65 yrs & older) (380)
400     TM- Ego Identity Process Questionnaire
401     TM- Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support
402     TM- Self-Concealment Scale
403     TM- Self-Consciousness Scale-Revised: Social Anxiety subscale
404     MD- Empirical Study
405     MD- Quantitative Study
406     PT- Journal
407     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
408     AT- Journal Article
409     MT- Electronic
410     MA- Electronic; Print
411     RD- 20070924
412     CD- 20071210
413     AN- 2007-13638-009
414     FR- Y
415     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-13638-009&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
416    
417     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
418    
419    
420     Record: 9
421    
422     TI- The concavity effect is a compound of local and global effects.
423     AU- Vandekerckhove, Joachim
424     AU- Panis, Sven
425     AU- Wagemans, Johan
426     AF- Vandekerckhove, Joachim, joachim.vandekerckhove@psy.kuleuven.be, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
427     AF- Panis, Sven, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
428     AF- Wagemans, Johan, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
429     AD- Vandekerckhove, Joachim, University of Leuven, Department of Psychology, Tiensestraat 102, B-3000, Leuven, Belgium, joachim.vandekerckhove@psy.kuleuven.be
430     SO- Perception & Psychophysics
431     VI- 69
432     IP- 7
433     YR- 2007
434     PM- Oct 2007
435     PG- 1253-1260
436     SP- 1253
437     PU- US: Psychonomic Society
438     SN- 0031-5117, Print; 1532-5962, Electronic;
439     LA- English
440     KP- concavity effect
441     KP- global effects
442     KP- change detection paradigm
443     KP- contour
444     KP- concave regions
445     KP- stimulus object
446     AB- Using a change detection paradigm, Barenholtz, Cohen, Feldman, and Singh (2003) found that changes in concave regions of a contour are more easily detected than changes in convex regions. In a series of three experiments, we investigated this concavity effect using the same paradigm. We observed the effect in wire-like stimuli as well as in silhouettes (Experiment 1) and in complex, smoothed images as opposed to angular polygons (Experiment 2). We also observed a systematic effect of the magnitude of the change (Experiment 1). Furthermore, we find that the effect cannot be attributed to either local or global processing effects, but rather to a combination of both "mere" concaveness and an effect due to changes in the perceived part structure of the stimulus object (Experiment 3). For our data analysis, we used a nonparametric bootstrap method, which greatly increases sensitivity (compared to more traditional analyses like ANOVA). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
447     MJ- Form and Shape Perception; Signal Detection (Perception); Stimulus Parameters
448     CL- Visual Perception (2323)
449     PO- Human (10)
450     LO- Belgium
451     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
452     GR- This research was supported by a research grant from the University Research Council (OT/00/007) and from the Fund for Scientific Research Research (FWO-Vlaanderen G.0189.02) to J.W. The research also forms part of a larger research program funded by the University Research Council (GOA-TBA/2005/03)
453     MD- Empirical Study
454     MD- Quantitative Study
455     PT- Journal
456     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
457     AT- Journal Article
458     MT- Print
459     MA- Electronic; Print
460     RD- 20071217
461     AN- 2007-17908-017
462     FR- Y
463     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-17908-017&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
464    
465     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
466    
467    
468     Record: 10
469    
470     TI- Linear and nonlinear analysis of heart rate patterns associated with fetal behavioral states in the antepartum period.
471     AU- Gonçalves, Hernâni
472     AU- Bernardes, João
473     AU- Rocha, Ana Paula
474     AU- Ayres-de-Campos, Diogo
475     AF- Gonçalves, Hernâni, hernani.goncalves@fc.up.pt, Universidade do Porto, Faculdade de Ciencias, Departamento de Matematica Aplicada, Porto, Portugal
476     AF- Bernardes, João, Departamento de Ginecologia e Obstetricia, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade do Porto, Porto, Portugal
477     AF- Rocha, Ana Paula, Universidade do Porto, Faculdade de Ciencias, Departamento de Matematica Aplicada, Porto, Portugal
478     AF- Ayres-de-Campos, Diogo, Departamento de Ginecologia e Obstetricia, Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade do Porto, Porto, Portugal
479     AD- Gonçalves, Hernâni, Universidade do Porto, Faculdade de Ciencias, Departamento de Matematica Aplicada, Rua do Campo Alegre, 687, 4169-007, Porto, Portugal, hernani.goncalves@fc.up.pt
480     SO- Early Human Development
481     VI- 83
482     IP- 9
483     YR- 2007
484     PM- Sep 2007
485     PG- 585-591
486     SP- 585
487     PU- Netherlands: Elsevier Science
488     SN- 0378-3782, Print;
489     DO- 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.12.006
490     LA- English
491     KP- heart rate patterns
492     KP- fetal behavioral states
493     KP- antepartum period
494     KP- physiology
495     KP- pathology
496     AB- Background: Fetal behavioral states are important indicators of fetal physiology and pathology associated to typical fetal heart rate (FHR) patterns. Aim: To provide linear and nonlinear analysis of FHR patterns associated with fetal behavioral states regarding a better understanding of these states and patterns. Study design and subjects: Fifty FHR tracings from normal term pregnancies with a median duration of 40.3 min were acquired with the SisPorto® 2.01 system for computerized analysis of cardiotocograms. Each tracing was divided into consecutive 10-minute segments and each segment was classified by two experts as pattern A, B, C or D. Outcome measures: Linear and nonlinear indices were computed in each segment, namely mean FHR, long-term irregularity index (LTI), very low (VLF), low (LF) and high (HF) frequency spectral indices, approximate entropy (ApEn) and sample entropy (SampEn). Kappa statistic (κ) and proportions of agreement (Pa) were used for assessment of inter-observer agreement. Bootstrap percentile confidence intervals and nonparametric statistical tests were calculated for statistical inference. Results: Overall agreement between experts in pattern classification was good to excellent with values for κ and Pa of 0.74 (95% CI: 0.64-0.94) and 0.94 (95% CI: 0.92-0.96), respectively. Most linear domain indices increased significantly with rising fetal activity whereas the opposite occurred with nonlinear indices, except for SampEn(2, 0.1). LF/(MF + HF) ratio also significantly increased with fetal activity, denoting an increased sympatho-vagal balance. Conclusions: Results support the hypothesis that entropy and linear variability indices measure different FHR features. FHR patterns associated with active sleep (B) and active wakefulness (D) evidenced more signs of autonomous nervous system activity, with sympatho-vagal imbalance, and less signs related to complexity or irregularity control systems than patterns associated with calm sleep (A) and calm wakefulness (C). (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
497     MJ- Heart Rate; Pathology; Physiology; Prenatal Development
498     MN- Behavior
499     CL- Physiological Processes (2540)
500     CL- Developmental Psychology (2800)
501     PO- Human (10)
502     GR- The authors acknowledge project POSI/CPS/40153/2001 funded by FCT (Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia, Portugal)
503     MD- Empirical Study
504     MD- Quantitative Study
505     PT- Journal
506     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
507     AT- Journal Article
508     MT- Electronic
509     MA- Electronic; Print
510     RD- 20071210
511     AN- 2007-13846-002
512     FR- Y
513     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-13846-002&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
514    
515     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
516    
517    
518     Record: 11
519    
520     TI- The cost of borderline personality disorder: Societal cost of illness in BPD-patients.
521     AU- van Asselt, A. D. I.
522     AU- Dirksen, C. D.
523     AU- Arntz, A.
524     AU- Severens, J. L.
525     AF- van Asselt, A. D. I., avas@kemta.azm.nl, Department of Clinical Epidemiology, University Hospital Maastricht, Maastricht, Netherlands
526     AF- Dirksen, C. D., Department of Clinical Epidemiology, University Hospital Maastricht, Maastricht, Netherlands
527     AF- Arntz, A., Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
528     AF- Severens, J. L., Department of Clinical Epidemiology, University Hospital Maastricht, Maastricht, Netherlands
529     AD- van Asselt, A. D. I., Department of Clinical Epidemiology and Medical Technology Assessment, University Hospital Maastricht, P.O. Box 5800, 6202 AZ, Maastricht, Netherlands, avas@kemta.azm.nl
530     SO- European Psychiatry
531     S2- Psychiatrie & Psychobiologie
532     VI- 22
533     IP- 6
534     YR- 2007
535     PM- Sep 2007
536     PG- 354-361
537     SP- 354
538     PU- Netherlands: Elsevier Science
539     SN- 0924-9338, Print;
540     DO- 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.04.001
541     LA- English
542     KP- borderline personality disorder
543     KP- illness cost
544     AB- Background: Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a highly prevalent, chronic condition. Because of its very problematic nature BPD is expected to be associated with substantial societal costs, although this has never been comprehensively assessed. Objective: Estimate the societal cost of BPD in the Netherlands. Study Design: We used a prevalence-based bottom-up approach with a sample of 88 BPD patients who enrolled in a multicenter clinical trial comparing two kinds of outpatient psychotherapy. Costs were assessed by means of a structured interview, covering all healthcare costs, medication, informal care, productivity losses, and out-of-pocket expenses. Only BPD-related costs were included. All costs were expressed in Euros for the year 2000. A bootstrap procedure was performed to determine statistical uncertainty. Patients: All patients had been diagnosed with BPD using DSM-IV criteria. Mean age was 30.5years and 92% was female. Results: Based on a prevalence of 1.1% and an adult population of 11,990,942, we derived that there were 131,900 BPD patients in the Netherlands. Total bootstrapped yearly cost of illness was €2,222,763,789 (€1, 372, 412, 403-€3, 260, 248, 300), only 22% was healthcare-related. Costs per patient were €16,852. Conclusions: Although healthcare costs of non-institutionalized Borderline patients might not be disproportionate, total societal costs are substantial. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
545     MJ- Borderline Personality Disorder; Costs and Cost Analysis; Health Care Costs; Society
546     MN- Economics
547     CL- Personality Disorders (3217)
548     PO- Human (10)
549     PO- Male (30)
550     PO- Female (40)
551     LO- Netherlands
552     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
553     AG- Young Adulthood (18-29 yrs) (320)
554     AG- Thirties (30-39 yrs) (340)
555     AG- Middle Age (40-64 yrs) (360)
556     TM- Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders
557     GR- The present study was supported by grant no. OG 97-002 from the fund for evaluative research in medicine of the Dutch Healthcare Insurance Board
558     MD- Empirical Study
559     MD- Quantitative Study
560     PT- Journal
561     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
562     AT- Journal Article
563     MT- Electronic
564     MA- Electronic; Print
565     RD- 20071022
566     AN- 2007-14025-004
567     FR- Y
568     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-14025-004&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
569    
570     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
571    
572    
573     Record: 12
574    
575     TI- The association between pathological gambling and attempted suicide: Findings from a national survey in Canada.
576     AU- Newman, Stephen C.
577     AU- Thompson, Angus H.
578     AF- Newman, Stephen C., stephen.newman@ualberta.ca, Department of Psychiatry, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
579     AF- Thompson, Angus H., Department of Public Health Sciences, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada
580     AD- Newman, Stephen C., Department of Psychiatry, Mackenzie Centre, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada, T6G 2B7, stephen.newman@ualberta.ca
581     SO- The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry / La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie
582     S2- The Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal / La Revue de l'Association des psychiatres du Canada
583     VI- 52
584     IP- 9
585     YR- 2007
586     PM- Sep 2007
587     PG- 605-612
588     SP- 605
589     PU- Canada: Canadian Psychiatric Assn
590     SN- 0706-7437, Print; 1497-0015, Electronic;
591     LA- English
592     KP- pathological gambling
593     KP- attempted suicide
594     KP- Canada
595     KP- mental health care
596     KP- alcohol dependence
597     KP- sociodemographic variables
598     AB- Objective: To examine the association between pathological gambling (PG) and attempted suicide in a nationally representative sample of Canadians. Methods: Data came from the Canadian Community Health Survey, Cycle 1.2, conducted in 2002, in which 36,984 subjects, aged 15 years or older, were interviewed. Logistic regression was performed with attempted suicide (in the past year) as the dependent variable. The independent variables were PG, major depression, alcohol dependence, drug dependence, and mental health care (in the past year), as well as a range of sociodemographic variables. Survey weights and bootstrap methods were used to account for the complex survey design. Results: In the final logistic regression model, which included terms for PG, major depression, alcohol dependence, and mental health care, as well as age, sex, education, and income, the odds ratio for PG and attempted suicide was 3.43 (95% confidence interval, 1.37 to 8.60). Conclusions: PG (in the past year) and attempted suicide (in the past year) are associated in a nationally representative sample of Canadians. However, it is not possible to say from these data whether this represents a causal relation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
599     MJ- Alcohol Abuse; Attempted Suicide; Drug Dependency; Major Depression; Pathological Gambling
600     MN- Demographic Characteristics; Mental Health Services
601     CL- Behavior Disorders & Antisocial Behavior (3230)
602     PO- Human (10)
603     PO- Male (30)
604     PO- Female (40)
605     LO- Canada
606     AG- Adolescence (13-17 yrs) (200)
607     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
608     AG- Young Adulthood (18-29 yrs) (320)
609     AG- Thirties (30-39 yrs) (340)
610     AG- Middle Age (40-64 yrs) (360)
611     AG- Aged (65 yrs & older) (380)
612     GR- This research was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research
613     CF- Mental Health Research Showcase, Oct, 2006, Banff, AB, Canada
614     CN- Portions of this paper were previously presented at the aforementioned conference.
615     MD- Empirical Study
616     MD- Quantitative Study
617     PT- Journal
618     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
619     AT- Journal Article
620     MT- Electronic
621     MA- Electronic; Print
622     RD- 20080114
623     AN- 2007-14432-009
624     FR- Y
625     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-14432-009&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
626    
627     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
628    
629    
630     Record: 13
631    
632     TI- Epidemiology of youth gambling problems in Canada: A national prevalence study.
633     AU- Huang, Jiun-Hau
634     AU- Boyer, Richard
635     AF- Huang, Jiun-Hau, jiun-hau.huang@umontreal.ca, Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, University of Montreal, Montreal, PQ, Canada
636     AF- Boyer, Richard, Social Psychiatry Unit, Fernand-Seguin Research Centre, Louis-H Lafontaine Hospital, Montreal, PQ, US
637     AD- Huang, Jiun-Hau, Social Psychiatry Unit, Fernand-Seguin Research Centre, Louis-H Lafontaine Hospital, 7331 Rue Hochelaga, Unit 218, Montreal, PQ, Canada, H1N 3V2, jiun-hau.huang@umontreal.ca
638     SO- The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry / La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie
639     S2- The Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal / La Revue de l'Association des psychiatres du Canada
640     VI- 52
641     IP- 10
642     YR- 2007
643     PM- Sep 2007
644     PG- 657-665
645     SP- 657
646     PU- Canada: Canadian Psychiatric Assn
647     SN- 0706-7437, Print; 1497-0015, Electronic;
648     LA- English
649     KP- epidemiology
650     KP- youth gambling problems
651     KP- Canada
652     KP- geographic region
653     KP- sex differences
654     AB- Objectives: To describe the epidemiology of gambling problems among youth aged 15 to 24 years in Canada and to examine whether these gambling prevalence patterns differ by sex and (or) by geographic region. Method: We used data from The Canadian Community Health Survey: Mental Health and Well-Being. Gambling problems were determined according to the Canadian Problem Gambling Index. All prevalence estimates used appropriate sampling weights and bootstrap variance estimation procedures developed by Statistics Canada. Multivariate logistic regression modelling was also employed to supplement the above prevalence comparisons by age, sex, and region. Results: Among Canadian youth aged 15 to 24 years (n = 5666), 61.35% gambled in the past 12 months and the national prevalence of moderate-risk or problem gambling was 2.22% (3.30% in male respondents and 1.10% in female respondents). Male respondents had significantly higher prevalence of gambling problems than female respondents. Regional prevalence estimates of youth moderate-risk or problem gambling were 1.37% in British Columbia, 2.17% in the Prairie provinces, 2.75% in Ontario, 2.12% in Quebec, and 1.71% in the Atlantic provinces. Conclusions: Youth, particularly young men, are at greater risk for gambling problems than adults. More prevention and research efforts are also needed to address the observed sex differences and interregional variability in the prevalence of gambling problems among youth. The national prevalence estimates from this study provide important baseline data against which future cohorts of Canadians can be monitored and measured. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
655     MJ- Epidemiology; Geography; Human Sex Differences; Pathological Gambling
656     CL- Behavior Disorders & Antisocial Behavior (3230)
657     PO- Human (10)
658     PO- Male (30)
659     PO- Female (40)
660     LO- Canada
661     AG- Adolescence (13-17 yrs) (200)
662     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
663     AG- Young Adulthood (18-29 yrs) (320)
664     TM- Canadian Problem Gambling Index
665     GR- This study was supported by a special grant from the Ministère de la Santé et des Services sociaux of Québec to Dr Boyer and by a postdoctoral research fellowship awarded to Dr Huang by the Centre de recherche Fernand-Seguin affiliated with the Hôpital Louis-H Lafontaine and the Université de Montréal
666     MD- Empirical Study
667     MD- Quantitative Study
668     PT- Journal
669     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
670     AT- Journal Article
671     MT- Electronic
672     MA- Electronic; Print
673     RD- 20080107
674     AN- 2007-16610-005
675     FR- Y
676     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-16610-005&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
677    
678     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
679    
680    
681     Record: 14
682    
683     TI- Single-word semantic judgements in semantic dementia: Do phonology and grammatical class count?
684     AU- Reilly, Jamie
685     AU- Cross, Katy
686     AU- Troiani, Vanessa
687     AU- Grossman, Murray
688     AF- Reilly, Jamie, reillyjj@mail.med.upenn.edu, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, US
689     AF- Cross, Katy, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, US
690     AF- Troiani, Vanessa, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, US
691     AF- Grossman, Murray, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, US
692     AD- Reilly, Jamie, Department of Neurology, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine, Hospital of University of Pennsylvania, 3 Gates Building, 3400 Spruce Street, Philadelphia, PA, US, 19104-4283, reillyjj@mail.med.upenn.edu
693     SO- Aphasiology
694     VI- 21
695     IP- 6
696     YR- 2007
697     PM- Aug 2007
698     PG- 558-569
699     SP- 558
700     PU- United Kingdom: Taylor & Francis
701     SN- 0268-7038, Print; 1464-5041, Electronic;
702     LA- English
703     KP- semantic judgements
704     KP- semantic dementia
705     KP- phonology
706     KP- syntactic processing
707     KP- acoustics
708     KP- phonetics
709     KP- nouns
710     KP- verbs
711     KP- lexical judgements
712     AB- Background: Listeners make active use of phonological regularities such as word length to facilitate higher-level syntactic and semantic processing. For example, nouns are longer than verbs, and abstract words are longer than concrete words. Patients with semantic dementia (SD) experience conceptual loss with preserved syntax and phonology. The extent to which patients with SD exploit phonological regularities to support language processing remains unclear. Aims: We examined the ability of patients with SD (1) to perceive subtle acoustic-phonetic distinctions in English, and (2) to bootstrap their accuracy of lexical-semantic and syntactic judgements from regularities in the phonological forms of English nouns and verbs. Methods and Procedures: Four patients with SD made minimal pair judgements (same/ different) for auditorily presented stimuli selectively varied by voice, place, or manner of the initial consonant (e.g., pa -ba). In Experiment 2 patients made forced-choice semantic judgements (abstract or concrete) for single words varied by (1) concreteness (abstract or concrete); (2) grammatical class (noun or verb); and (3) word length (one- or three-syllable words). Outcomes and Results: The most semantically impaired patients paradoxically showed the highest accuracy of minimal pair phonologic discrimination. Judgements of word concreteness were less accurate for verbs than nouns. Among verbs, accuracy was worse for concrete than abstract items (e.g., eat was worse than think). Patients were more likely to misclassify longer concrete words (e.g., professor) as abstract, demonstrating sensitivity to an underlying phonologically mediated semantic property in English. Conclusions: Single-word semantic judgements were sensitive to both grammatical class and phonological properties of the words being evaluated. Theoretical and clinical implications are addressed in the context of an anatomically constrained model of SD that assumes increasing reliance on phonology as lexical-semantic knowledge degrades. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
713     MJ- Acoustics; Dementia; Lexical Decision; Semantics; Syntax
714     MN- Judgment; Nouns; Phonetics; Verbs
715     CL- Neurological Disorders & Brain Damage (3297)
716     PO- Human (10)
717     PO- Male (30)
718     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
719     AG- Middle Age (40-64 yrs) (360)
720     AG- Aged (65 yrs & older) (380)
721     TM- Pyramids and Palm Trees Test
722     TM- Mini Mental State Examination
723     TM- Boston Naming Test
724     MD- Empirical Study
725     MD- Quantitative Study
726     PT- Journal
727     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
728     AT- Journal Article
729     MT- Electronic
730     MA- Electronic; Print
731     RD- 20071119
732     AN- 2007-13670-004
733     FR- Y
734     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-13670-004&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
735    
736     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
737    
738    
739     Record: 15
740    
741     TI- Obligatory exercise and eating pathology in college females: Replication and development of a structural model.
742     AU- Thome, Jennifer L.
743     AU- Espelage, Dorothy L.
744     AF- Thome, Jennifer L., University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, IL, US
745     AF- Espelage, Dorothy L., espelage@uiuc.edu, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, IL, US
746     AD- Espelage, Dorothy L., espelage@uiuc.edu
747     SO- Eating Behaviors
748     VI- 8
749     IP- 3
750     YR- 2007
751     PM- Aug 2007
752     PG- 334-349
753     SP- 334
754     PU- Netherlands: Elsevier Science
755     SN- 1471-0153, Print;
756     DO- 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.11.009
757     LA- English
758     KP- obligatory exercise
759     KP- restrictive eating
760     KP- personality structural model
761     KP- college females
762     AB- Although disordered eating behaviors and obligatory exercise are relatively common among undergraduate females, many questions about their etiology remain unanswered. In the current study, structural equation modeling was used to investigate whether a model of personality, exercise attitudes, and exercise behavior that was previously developed with a clinical sample [Davis, C., Katzman, D.K., & Kirsh, C. (1999). Compulsive physical activity in adolescents with anorexia nervosa: A psychobehavioral spiral of pathology. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 336-342, see record 1999-05765-002] would also fit for a sample of non-clinical college females (n = 599). Further, a second model extended the first model to predict exercise behavior and eating/weight restriction. Results indicated that the original structural model fit the non-clinical sample extremely well. Specifically, addictiveness and obsessive-compulsiveness were associated with obligatory attitudes toward exercise, which was then associated with exercise behavior. In the second model, obligatory attitudes toward exercise were equally associated with eating/weight restriction as it was with exercise behavior. In a third model, bootstrapping analyses demonstrated that while obligatory exercise was directly associated with eating and weight restriction, this association was partially mediated by weight-related reasons for exercise. These data suggest that the aforementioned variables are predictive of obligatory exercise and eating pathology in non-clinical samples, and that reasons for exercise is important in understanding the complex relations among disordered eating and exercise attitudes and behaviors. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
763     MJ- Anorexia Nervosa; Cognitions; Exercise; Personality Traits
764     MN- College Students; Human Females; Personality Correlates; Structural Equation Modeling
765     CL- Eating Disorders (3260)
766     PO- Human (10)
767     PO- Female (40)
768     LO- US
769     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
770     TM- Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Addiction Scale
771     TM- Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory
772     TM- Maudsley Obsessional-Compulsive Inventory
773     TM- Obligatory Exercise Questionnaire
774     TM- Commitment to Exercise Scale
775     TM- Godin Leisure-Time Questionnaire
776     TM- Aerobics Center Longitudinal Study Physical Activity Questionnaire
777     TM- Reasons for Exercise Inventory
778     TM- Exercise Dependence Questionnaire
779     TM- Eating Disorder Inventory-2
780     TM- NEO Personality Inventory-Revised
781     TM- Eating Attitudes Test
782     MD- Empirical Study
783     MD- Experimental Replication
784     MD- Quantitative Study
785     PT- Journal
786     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
787     AT- Journal Article
788     MT- Electronic
789     MA- Electronic; Print
790     RD- 20070716
791     AN- 2007-10399-008
792     FR- Y
793     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-10399-008&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
794    
795     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
796    
797    
798     Record: 16
799    
800     TI- Factors associated with antipsychotic dosing in psychiatric inpatients: A prospective study.
801     AU- Barbui, Corrado
802     AU- Biancosino, Bruno
803     AU- Esposito, Eleonora
804     AU- Marmai, Luciana
805     AU- Donà, Silvia
806     AU- Grassi, Luigi
807     AF- Barbui, Corrado, corrado.barbui@univr.it, Section of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, Department of Medicine, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
808     AF- Biancosino, Bruno, Section of Psychiatry, Department of Medical Sciences of Communication and Behaviour, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
809     AF- Esposito, Eleonora, Section of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, Department of Medicine, University of Verona, Verona, Italy
810     AF- Marmai, Luciana, Section of Psychiatry, Department of Medical Sciences of Communication and Behaviour, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
811     AF- Donà, Silvia, Section of Psychiatry, Department of Medical Sciences of Communication and Behaviour, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
812     AF- Grassi, Luigi, Section of Psychiatry, Department of Medical Sciences of Communication and Behaviour, University of Ferrara, Ferrara, Italy
813     AD- Barbui, Corrado, Department of Medicine and Public Health, Section of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, University of Verona, Ospedale Policlinico, 37134, Verona, Italy, corrado.barbui@univr.it
814     SO- International Clinical Psychopharmacology
815     VI- 22
816     IP- 4
817     YR- 2007
818     PM- Jul 2007
819     PG- 221-225
820     SP- 221
821     PU- US: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
822     SN- 0268-1315, Print; 1473-5857, Electronic;
823     DO- 10.1097/YIC.0b013e3281084ea8
824     LA- English
825     KP- antipsychotic dosing
826     KP- psychiatric inpatients
827     KP- excessive dosing
828     AB- The persistent use of doses in excess of recommended levels is associated with increased risks of adverse reactions without evidence of additional benefits. Such treatment modality was evaluated in hospitalized psychiatric patients. During a 6-year recruitment period, a consecutive series of psychiatric inpatients receiving antipsychotic therapy were included. At admission, sociodemographic and clinical data, including antipsychotic drug use, were collected, and the 18-item version of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale was administered. At discharge, data on antipsychotic drug therapy were collected. Prescribed daily doses were converted into multiples of the defined daily doses. Using a cut-off score of a prescribed daily dose/defined daily dose as a ratio of more than 1.5 both at admission and at discharge assessments, a total of 62 (15.4%) patients persistently received high antipsychotic dose. With less stringent criteria (prescribed daily dose/defined daily dose as a ratio of more than 2), however, only 4.4% of the entire sample was persistently exposed to high antipsychotic doses. Bootstrapped linear regression analysis revealed that positive symptoms were positively associated with high antipsychotic dose, whereas negative symptoms were negatively associated with high antipsychotic dose. Antipsychotic polypharmacy at admission was the strongest predictor of persistently receiving antipsychotic doses in excess of recommended levels. This study showed that the use of high antipsychotic dosing is not an occasional event. Clinicians should consider that concurrent prescribing of two or more antipsychotic agents increases the probability of administering excessive dosing in the long-term. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
829     MJ- Drug Dosages; Neuroleptic Drugs; Psychiatric Patients
830     CL- Clinical Psychopharmacology (3340)
831     PO- Human (10)
832     PO- Male (30)
833     PO- Female (40)
834     PO- Inpatient (50)
835     LO- Italy
836     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
837     TM- Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale
838     MD- Empirical Study
839     MD- Longitudinal Study
840     MD- Prospective Study
841     MD- Quantitative Study
842     PT- Journal
843     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
844     AT- Journal Article
845     MT- Electronic
846     MA- Electronic; Print
847     RD- 20070723
848     AN- 2007-08010-006
849     FR- Y
850     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-08010-006&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
851    
852     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
853    
854    
855     Record: 17
856    
857     TI- Machine learning approach to color constancy.
858     AU- Agarwal, Vivek
859     AU- Gribok, Andrei V.
860     AU- Abidi, Mongi A.
861     AF- Agarwal, Vivek, agarwal1@purdue.edu, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, US
862     AF- Gribok, Andrei V., agribok@bioanalysis.org, BHSAI/MRMC, Attn: MCMR-ZB-T, Fort Detrick, MD, US
863     AF- Abidi, Mongi A., Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN, US
864     AD- Agarwal, Vivek, School of Nuclear Engineering, Purdue University, 400 Central Drive, West Lafayette, IN, US, 47907, agarwal1@purdue.edu
865     SO- Neural Networks
866     VI- 20
867     IP- 5
868     YR- 2007
869     PM- Jul 2007
870     PG- 559-563
871     SP- 559
872     PU- Netherlands: Elsevier Science
873     SN- 0893-6080, Print;
874     DO- 10.1016/j.neunet.2007.02.004
875     LA- English
876     KP- machine learning approach
877     KP- color constancy algorithms
878     KP- computer vision
879     KP- neural networks
880     KP- support vector regression
881     KP- video tracking application
882     AB- A number of machine learning (ML) techniques have recently been proposed to solve color constancy problem in computer vision. Neural networks (NNs) and support vector regression (SVR) in particular, have been shown to outperform many traditional color constancy algorithms. However, neither neural networks nor SVR were compared to simpler regression tools in those studies. In this article, we present results obtained with a linear technique known as ridge regression (RR) and show that it performs better than NNs, SVR, and gray world (GW) algorithm on the same dataset. We also perform uncertainty analysis for NNs, SVR, and RR using bootstrapping and show that ridge regression and SVR are more consistent than neural networks. The shorter training time and single parameter optimization of the proposed approach provides a potential scope for real time video tracking application. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
883     MJ- Algorithms; Color Constancy; Machine Learning; Neural Networks; Tracking
884     MN- Computer Applications; Vision
885     CL- Intelligent Systems (4100)
886     PO- Human (10)
887     GR- This work is supported by the University Research Program in Robotics under grant DE-FG52-2004NA25589 by DOE
888     PT- Journal
889     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
890     AT- Journal Article
891     MT- Electronic
892     MA- Electronic; Print
893     RD- 20071112
894     AN- 2007-11911-001
895     FR- Y
896     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-11911-001&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
897    
898     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
899    
900    
901     Record: 18
902    
903     TI- The symbol detachment problem.
904     AU- Pezzulo, Giovanni
905     AU- Castelfranchi, Cristiano
906     AF- Pezzulo, Giovanni, giovanni.pezzulo@istc.cnr.it, Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, CNR, Rome, Italy
907     AF- Castelfranchi, Cristiano, cristiano.castelfranchi@istc.cnr.it, Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, CNR, Rome, Italy
908     AD- Pezzulo, Giovanni, Istituto di Scienze e Tecnologie della Cognizione, CNR, Via S. Martino della Battaglia, 44, 00185, Rome, Italy, giovanni.pezzulo@istc.cnr.it
909     SO- Cognitive Processing
910     VI- 8
911     IP- 2
912     YR- 2007
913     PM- Jun 2007
914     PG- 115-131
915     SP- 115
916     PU- Germany: Springer
917     SN- 1612-4782, Print; 1612-4790, Electronic;
918     DO- 10.1007/s10339-007-0164-0
919     LA- English
920     KP- symbol detachment problem
921     KP- cognition
922     KP- mental state
923     KP- expectations
924     KP- goals
925     AB- In situated and embodied approaches it is commonly assumed that the dynamics of sensorimotor engagement between an adaptive agent and its environment are crucial in understanding natural cognition. This perspective permits to address the symbol grounding problem, since the aboutness of any mental state arising during agent-environment engagement is guaranteed by their continuous coupling. However, cognitive agents are also able to formulate representations that are detached from the current state of affairs, such as expectations and goals. Moreover, they can act on their representations before--or instead of--acting directly on the environment, for example building the plan of a bridge and not directly the bridge. On the basis of representations, actions such as planning, remembering or imagining are possible that are disengaged from the current sensorimotor cycle, and often functional to future-oriented conducts. A new problem thus has to be acknowledged, the symbol detachment problem: how and why do situated agents develop representations that are detached from their current sensorimotor interaction, but nevertheless preserve grounding and aboutness? How do cognitive agents progressively acquire a range of capabilities permitting them to deal not only with the current situation but also with alternative, in particular future states of affairs? How do they develop the capability of acting on their representations instead of acting directly on the world? In a theoretical and developmental perspective, we propose that anticipation plays a crucial role in the detachment process: anticipatory representations, originally detached from the sensorimotor cycle for the sake of action control, are successively exapted for bootstrapping increasingly complex cognitive capabilities. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
926     MJ- Cognition; Cognitive Ability; Expectations; Goals
927     CL- Cognitive Processes (2340)
928     PO- Human (10)
929     GR- This work is supported by the EU-funded project MindRACES: from Reactive to Anticipatory Cognitive Embodied Systems (FP6-511931)
930     PT- Journal
931     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
932     AT- Journal Article
933     MT- Print
934     MA- Electronic; Print
935     RD- 20070917
936     AN- 2007-08876-006
937     FR- Y
938     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-08876-006&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
939    
940     ------------------------------------------------------------------------
941    
942    
943     Record: 19
944    
945     TI- The relationship between text comprehension and second language incidental vocabulary acquisition: A matter of topic familiarity?
946     AU- Pulido, Diana
947     AF- Pulido, Diana, pulidod@msu.edu, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, US
948     AD- Pulido, Diana, Department of Linguistics and Languages, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, US, 48824-1027, pulidod@msu.edu
949     SO- Language Learning
950     VI- 57
951     IP- 1
952     YR- 2007
953     PM- Jun 2007
954     PG- 155-199
955     SP- 155
956     PU- United Kingdom: Blackwell Publishing
957     SN- 0023-8333, Print; 1467-9922, Electronic;
958     DO- 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2007.00415.x
959     LA- English
960     KP- lexical input processing outcomes
961     KP- text comprehension
962     KP- second language incidental vocabulary acquisition
963     KP- topic familiarity
964     AB- The study reported in this article describes second language (L2) vocabulary learning outcomes associated with adult L2 reading comprehension processes, thus connecting L2 learning with the complex cognitive and linguistic processing involved in reading. The study aimed to determine whether background knowledge moderated the relationship between passage comprehension and lexical input processing outcomes, such as intake and receptive gain and retention of target-word meanings. The primary theoretical contribution concerns the nature of the relationships that obtained between passage comprehension and lexical input processing. Overall, the results suggest that as learners become more efficient in engaging in the various processing activities required during L2 reading, they experience greater memory for linguistic elements encountered during reading, such as orthographic forms and semantic aspects of new lexical items. The results support connectionist models of L2 reading and conclusions concerning efficiency in lower level text processing (e.g., see Koda, 2005; Nassaji, 2002). Reading is a complex cognitive activity, involving simultaneous linguistic processing such as pattern recognition, letter identification, lexical access, concept activation, syntactic analysis, propositional encoding, sentence comprehension, and intersentence integration, as well as the activation of prior knowledge, information storage, and comprehension monitoring. The prior knowledge that is accessed is largely determined by the quality of the textbase that is constructed during reading, which is affected by the individual's efficiency in carrying out the various text-processing operations listed above. The finding that background knowledge did not moderate the relationship between comprehension and receptive retention of meaning provides further support for the robust role of text-processing efficiency in bootstrapping processes, such as those involved in mapping new linguistic forms to familiar concepts already stored in memory. The secondary finding with regard to intake of the target words corroborates the main finding, demonstrating that efficient text processing skills are also essential to other aspects of lexical input processing, such as establishing and retrieving the connections between new linguistic forms and the specific contexts in which they were encountered. The study also expands upon previous reading and research in the area of methodological innovations applied to incidental learning research paradigms, for instance, by measuring intake and episodic memory and the relationships between text processing and language learning outcomes. In addition, through the concurrent investigation of the impact of several factors on lexical input processing, the study sets the stage for more complex modeling of the processes that contribute to L2 development. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2007 APA, all rights reserved) (from the journal abstract)
965     MJ- Foreign Languages; Reading Comprehension; Vocabulary
966     MN- Incidental Learning; Linguistics; Sentence Comprehension; Word Meaning
967     CL- Linguistics & Language & Speech (2720)
968     PO- Human (10)
969     AG- Adulthood (18 yrs & older) (300)
970     CF- Form-Meaning Connections in Second Language Acquisition (FMSLA), Feb, 2002, Chicago, IL, US
971     CN- Some of the results of the present study were presented in a paper at the aforementioned conference.
972     MD- Empirical Study
973     MD- Quantitative Study
974     PT- Journal
975     PT- Peer Reviewed Journal
976     AT- Journal Article
977     MT- Electronic
978     MA- Electronic; Print
979     RD- 20070730
980     AN- 2007-09429-005
981     FR- Y
982     UR- http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-09429-005&loginpage=login.asp&site=ehost-live&scope=site
983    
984     Back
985     <javascript:__doPostBack('ctl00$MainContentArea$deliveryPrintSaveControl$backButtonBottom$lnkBack','')>
986    
987    

  ViewVC Help
Powered by ViewVC 1.1.26